A Genetically Informed Study of the Intergenerational
Transmission of Marital Instability
Brian M. D’Onofrio,
Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Indiana University, 1101 E, 10th Street,
Bloomington, IN 47405
Eric Turkheimer,
The Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, 102 Gilmer Hall, P.O. Box 400400,
Charlottesville, VA 22904
Robert E. Emery,
The Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, 102 Gilmer Hall, P.O. Box 400400,
Charlottesville, VA 22904
K. Paige Harden,
The Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, 102 Gilmer Hall, P.O. Box 400400,
Charlottesville, VA 22904
Wendy S. Slutske,
Department of Psychological Sciences, University of Missouri, 210 McAlester Hall, Columbia, MO
65211
Andrew C. Heath,
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, Campus Box 8134, St.
Louis, MO 63110
Pamela A. F. Madden, and
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, Campus Box 8134, St.
Louis, MO 63110
Nicholas G. Martin
Royal Brisbane Hospital Post Office, Queensland Institute of Medical Research, 300 Herston
Road, Q 4029, Brisbane, Australia
Brian M. D’Onofrio: bmdonofr@indiana.edu
Abstract
Environmental or genetic influences, or both could account for the increased risk of divorce
among the offspring of separated parents. Previous studies have used covariates to statistically
control for confounds, but the present research is the first genetically informed study of the topic.
The investigation used the Children of Twins Design with twins, their spouses, and their young
adult offspring (n = 2,310) from the Australian Twin Registry to test whether selection on the
basis of genetic or shared environment factors accounted for part of the inter-generational
association. The analyses also controlled for measured characteristics of both parents. The results
suggest that both environmentally mediated and genetic risk account for the intergenerational
transmission, supporting the roles of both selection and causation.
Keywords
divorce; intergenerational transmission; multilevel models; selection effects
NIH Public Access
Author Manuscript
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
Published in final edited form as:
J Marriage Fam. 2007 August 1; 69(3): 793–809. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3737.2007.00406.x.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
The intergenerational transmission of divorce has been found in large samples throughout
the world (see Pryor & Rogers, 2001), including the United States (Greenstein, 1995) and
Australia (Rodgers, 1996). In fact, research has consistently shown that parental divorce is
one of the strongest predictors of marital instability (Amato, 1999). Researchers have sought
to identify possible mediators of the association between parental and offspring marital
instability. Various studies have examined factors such as age at first marriage and
cohabitation, differences in perspectives on the costs and benefits associated with marriage,
modeling of unsuccessful relationship and interpersonal skills, mate selection, and lower
commitment to marriage in the offspring of divorced parents (review in Fine & Harvey,
2006). Despite their unique foci, all these efforts share one general assumption: Parental
divorce increases the risk for offspring marital instability through some causal mechanisms
(e.g., Amato, 2000). We refer to this view as the causal assumption.
It is possible, however, that the statistical relation between parental and offspring marital
instability is not causal. Selection factors that lead to marital instability in both generations
may be responsible for the association (Amato, 2000; Emery, 1999). Figure 1 presents a
graphical representation of two of the main classes of confounds when studying
intergenerational associations. First, common environmental factors may account for a
statistical relation between a parental and an offspring characteristic. For example, couples
living in poverty are more likely to separate, and socioeconomic status could be a third
variable that explains the intergenerational association. Statistical procedures can control for
possible environmental selection factors, such as socioeconomic status (reviews in Amato,
2000; Emery), but such analyses cannot control for environmental factors that have not been
measured or have been measured inaccurately. Further, no study has included in-depth
assessments of characteristics and psychopathology in both parents. This is a major
limitation, given that parental psychopathology is associated with changes in family
functioning, and the transmission of psychopathology could account for some of the
intergenerational transmission of divorce. Studies of the consequences of divorce that have
included measures of maternal psychopathology have underscored the important role of
selection factors (Capaldi & Patterson, 1991; Emery, Waldron, Kitzmann, & Aaron, 1999).
Therefore, exploring the risks for offspring relationship instability associated with the
psychopathology and demographic characteristics of both parents will provide a more
complete understanding of the processes responsible for the transmission of relationship
instability.
Second, genetic risks might also account for the association between parent and offspring
marital instability, an example of passive gene-environment correlation (Scarr &
McCartney, 1983). Passive gene-environment correlation occurs when genetic factors that
influence parental characteristics are passed down to the offspring and confer risk to the
children. In the most extreme case of genetic confounds, the association between the familial
risk factor and offspring adjustment is fully accounted for by shared genetic liability in both
generations, rendering the parental variable a marker for genetic selection rather than
environmental causation. Because twin studies have shown that genetic factors influence
divorce (D’Onofrio et al., 2005, in press; McGue & Lykken, 1992), there is a possibility that
genetic factors may confound the inter-generational transmission of divorce. Therefore,
exploring the possible role of genetic confounds is a critical research endeavor in exploring
family risk factors, such as parental divorce.
Although genes do not code for divorce, genetic factors may influence intermediate
characteristics, referred to as endophenotypes (Gottesman & Gould, 2003), that increase the
probability of divorce. For example, personality characteristics, such as neuroticism (Karney
& Bradbury, 1995) or antisocial behavior (Emery, 1999), are associated with increased risk
for divorce. Behavior genetic studies have consistently demonstrated that genetic factors
D’Onofrio et al. Page 2
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
account for variation in such traits (reviews in Bouchard & Loehlin, 2001; Rhee &
Waldman, 2002), and a twin study illustrated that personality characteristics account for a
portion of the genetic variation in divorce (Jockin, McGue, & Lykken, 1996). In light of the
genetic influences on divorce, Judith Harris has claimed, “Heredity … makes the children of
divorce more likely to fail in their own marriages. Parental divorce has no lasting effects on
the way children behave when they are not at home” (Begley, 1998).
We are considerably more cautious, however, in drawing conclusions about the role of
genetic factors explaining offspring adjustment problems associated with parental divorce.
Recent behavior genetic studies have found that genetic factors play a smaller role in
divorce than originally estimated (review in D’Onofrio et al., 2005). Furthermore, the source
of a risk factor can be separate from the mechanisms through which a risk factor influences
an offspring’s adjustment (Rutter, Silberg, & Simonoff, 1993). A risk factor, such as
divorce, could have an environmental effect (the risk mechanism) on children regardless of
whether genetic or environmental factors account for variation in divorce (the source of the
risk). Behavior genetic studies showing that genetic factors account for variability in divorce
raise the possibility that genetic factors account for the relations between parental divorce
and offspring adjustment, but this evidence does not substantiate the role. To our
knowledge, no investigation has yet explored the possibility that genetic factors confound
the intergenerational transmission of marital instability.
Because studies of the intergenerational transmission of divorce obviously cannot use
experimental designs, research must use methodologies that disentangle risk factors that
typically go together, especially genetic and environmental processes (Rutter, Pickles,
Murray, & Eaves, 2001). The need to incorporate behavior genetic designs in studies of risk
factors and intergenerational transmission has been echoed by family researchers (Booth,
Carver, & Granger, 2000) and developmental psychologists (Collins, Maccoby, Steinberg,
Hetherington, & Bornstein, 2000). Many genetically informative designs can be used to
explore environmental risk processes (review in Rutter et al., 2001). The current analyses
used the Children of Twins Design, an approach that utilizes comparison groups that differ
in their genetic and environmental risks associated with parental characteristics, such as
divorce (D’Onofrio et al., 2003, 2005; Gottesman & Bertelsen, 1989; Heath, Kendler, Eaves,
& Markell, 1985).
The Children of Twins Design
Most studies of the intergenerational transmission of marital instability have calculated the
difference between offspring from intact and divorced homes, referred to as the divorce
effect, by comparing offspring whose parents are divorced to unrelated offspring from intact
families. Statistical controls often are used to help account for the differences between the
unrelated families, but it is impossible to measure all potential selection factors. In the
Children of Twins Design, offspring of adult twins who are discordant for divorce (one
cotwin has divorced and the other has not) are compared. That is, offspring from divorced
families are compared to their cousins who did not experience the separation of their
parents, a within – twin family comparison (see Rodgers, Cleveland, van den Oord, &
Rowe, 2000, for an explanation of the advantages of making comparisons at multiple levels
of analysis). The comparison of cousins differentially exposed to parental divorce controls
for measured and unmeasured risk factors that all offspring in an extended family share.
Offspring of identical twins represent a unique comparison group. Because identical
(monozygotic) twins share 100% of their genetic makeup, the children of each twin are
genetically related to their twin parent and their aunt/uncle similarly (i.e., the offspring share
50% of their genetic makeup with their twin parent and their parent’s cotwin). The
comparison of offspring of discordant identical twins, therefore, contrasts cousins who share
D’Onofrio et al. Page 3
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
the same degree of genetic risk associated with divorce from their twin parents. As a result,
the divorce effect using children from identical twin pairs discordant for divorce is free from
genetic confounds associated with divorce in the twin parents. Another important and often
unrecognized advantage of the Children of Twins Design is that it controls for
environmental (measured and unmeasured) factors in the twin family that make the twins
similar because twins share their family environment as well as their genes. If the divorce
effect in discordant identical families is lower than the divorce effect using unrelated
comparisons, this suggests that selection factors, either genetic or shared environmental
confounds, account for part of the intergenerational transmission. See D’Onofrio et al.
(2005) for a graphical representation of the logic underlying the approach.
The Children of Twins Design can also help to separate common environmental and genetic
processes by comparing the offspring of identical and fraternal (dizygotic) twins discordant
for divorce. Offspring of identical twins are genetically related to each other as half siblings
(they share approximately 25% of their genes), whereas offspring of fraternal twins are
genetically related as cousins (sharing roughly 12.5% of their genes). Because genetic
influences are controlled more fully in the offspring of identical twins, a larger within – twin
family divorce effect (the comparison of offspring in intact and divorced households) in
fraternal twin families than identical twin families suggests that genetic factors are partly
responsible for the association. A lower divorce effect in offspring of identical twins than
fraternal twins would suggest that the selection factor would be correlated with genetic risk.
If there is no difference in the within – twin family divorce effects between identical and
fraternal families, shared environmental factors would be most salient because the salient
confounds would not correlate with genetic risk associated with divorce.
In summary, the Children of Twins Design is able to partition an intergenerational
association into three processes. First, the difference among offspring of discordant identical
twins provides an estimate of the processes specifically related to divorce within a nuclear
family (consistent with a causal connection). Second, small divorce effects in identical and
fraternal twin families imply that environmental factors that influence both generations
account for the intergenerational transmission (supporting selection effects). Third, a small
divorce effect in offspring of identical twins discordant for divorce but a larger divorce
estimate in fraternal twin families suggests a shared genetic pathway is responsible for the
increased divorce risk in the offspring (also supporting selection effects).
A qualification is in order. The design offers many advantages over solely relying on
statistical controls to account for possible confounds, but it does not control for every
possible environmental and genetic factor. The approach does not control for environmental
factors that only influence one twin and their offspring (D’Onofrio et al., 2003). The
influence of the spouse of the twins also is not accounted for in the design (Eaves, Silberg,
& Maes, 2005). The Children of Twins Design further assumes that there is no assortative
mating (Heath et al., 1985), the tendency for individuals to marry individuals who are
similar to them. The assumption can limit the ability of the design to differentiate between
environmental and genetic risk processes (see Discussion). Given these limitations,
including measures of nonshared environments and characteristics of the nontwin parents in
an attempt to account for these potential confounds and assumptions can provide a more
stringent test of the causal processes that account for the intergenerational transmission
(D’Onofrio et al., 2005; Rutter et al., 2001).
The current article sought to test whether the intergenerational transmission of divorce is
robust to the use of a quasiexperimental approach of controlling for possible confounds, as
well as the use of measured covariates. The present analyses, using a sample of adult twins,
their spouses, and their offspring from Australia, included detailed measures of possible
D’Onofrio et al. Page 4
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
selection factors in both parents to provide a more precise examination of selection effects.
The inclusion of the measured characteristics, particularly assessments of psychopathology
in both parents, represents a major advance over many of the existent studies of the
intergenerational transmission of divorce that have only included measures of mothers as
possible selection factors. The analyses would support an environmentally mediated causal
influence of parental marital instability on offspring instability if the statistical relation
between parents on offspring remained when comparing offspring of identical twins
discordant for divorce (a within – twin family comparison that accounts for unmeasured
environmental and genetic factors) and the use of statistical covariates from both parents to
account for possible confounds. If the relation between parents and offspring was reduced
by the use of the genetically informed design or the covariates, characteristics of parents or
their families correlated with parental divorce would partially or fully explain the
intergenerational transmission. Previous analyses of the Australia data set have found that
most measures of offspring adjustment related to parental divorce, such as drug, alcohol,
behavior, internalizing, and adjustment problems (D’Onofrio et al., 2005), are not the
consequences of genetic or environmental confounds, in as much as the design can control
for these selection factors. In contrast, the association between parental divorce and
cohabitation, a well-known risk factor for later relationship instability, was completely
accounted for by selection factors (D’Onofrio et al., 2006). A large Children of Twins study
in the United States also suggests that parental divorce may not cause increased depressive
symptoms; rather, shared genetic liability in the parents and offspring may account for the
association (D’Onofrio et al., in press). These findings underscore the importance of
studying the intergenerational transmission of marital instability in a quasiexperimental
design.
Method
Samples
Adult twins for the current analyses were drawn from the Australian National Twin
Register. The twins in the current cohort were first assessed in 1980 with a self-report
survey (n = 8,183 individual twins, 69% response rate; Jardine & Martin, 1984). In 1988, the
twins were assessed again with a mailed questionnaire (n = 6,327 individuals, 83% response
rate; Heath & Martin, 1994). Relatives of the twins (n = 14,421), including 3,318 spouses,
were also assessed with a similar questionnaire (Lake, Eaves, Maes, Heath, & Martin, 2000).
Finally, the twins and their spouses (86% response rate) were assessed via telephone
beginning in 1992 with a semistructured diagnostic interview (Heath et al., 1997). The study
included 5,889 individuals from the twin pairs and 3,844 of their spouses. The sample is
primarily White, consistent with the demographics of Australia. Tests for self-selection
biases in the longitudinal sample have found few detectable differences in terms of risk for
abnormal behavior (Heath et al., 1997).
The Children of Twins study was designed to examine the intergenerational transmission of
psychopathology and risk associated with divorce. Offspring of adult twins were selected
from a control group and three at-risk subgroups: (a) twins with a history of alcohol
dependence or conduct disorder, or both, (b) twins with a history of depression, and (c)
twins with a history of divorce. The adult twins were required to consent for the researchers
to contact their children, and the offspring also had to agree to participate. Approximately
85% (n = 1,409) of the targeted adult twins completed the screening, and 82% (n = 2,554) of
the possible offspring completed the telephone interview. The offspring ranged from 14 to
39 years old (M = 25.1 years), and 51% were female. Offspring over the age of 18 years old
(n = 2,334) were included in the current analyses. More details about the adult and offspring
samples are found elsewhere (D’Onofrio et al., 2005).
D’Onofrio et al. Page 5
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Measures
Characteristics of twins and their spouses—A lifetime history of the adult twins’
living arrangements was included in 1988 questionnaire, and all assessments included items
on current marital status and duration of the current status. On the basis of these items, a
lifetime history of divorce and marital separation (including separation from a cohabiting
relationship longer than 6 months) was calculated for each twin. Living together as though
married for 6 months or more is considered common law marriage in Australia; thus, we
refer to the relationships as marriages. Twin analyses indicated that additive genetic factors
accounted for 15% (95% CI = 5% – 19%) of the variation in marital instability (D’Onofrio
et al., 2005). Nonshared environmental factors, environmental influences that make siblings
and twins different, accounted for most of the variance (85%, 81% – 90%), with a minimal
role of environmental factors that equally influenced both twins (0%, 0% – 7%).
The twins and their spouses each completed the Semi-Structured Assessment for the
Genetics of Alcoholism (Bucholz et al., 1994) as part of the 1992 study. The semistructured
interview, developed on the basis of previously validated psychiatric research interviews,
has demonstrated moderate to high reliability (Bucholz et al., 1994), especially in Australia
(Slutske et al., 1998). The interview assessed various characteristics of the adult twins and
their spouses that have been shown to increase the risk of relationship instability (e.g., Fine
& Harvey, 2006). These parental characteristics were included in analyses as measured
covariates. Each parent’s age at the birth of their first child was calculated using the twins’
and their spouses’ reports of all their children’s dates of birth. Church attendance was
measured on a five-point Likert scale ranging from never to more than once a week. Highest
level of education was measured on a seven-point Likert scale ranging from less than 7
years’ schooling to university postgraduate training. Parental psychopathology was
measured with the number of lifetime symptoms of DSM-III-R diagnoses for conduct
disorder, alcohol abuse, and major depression; lifetime history of ever using an illegal drug;
and lifetime history of suicidality as measured on a five-point Likert scale ranging from no
thoughts or plans of suicide to a previous serious suicide attempt (Statham et al., 1998).
Spousal information was only included in the analyses if the spouse was the biological
parent of all the offspring in the current study. The twin and spousal information was
converted to maternal and paternal variables.
Offspring Relational Instability
The offspring also completed the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of
Alcoholism, which included a number of items assessing current marital status and history
of cohabitation. On the basis of these variables, a lifetime history of relationship instability
was calculated. Similar to the adult twins, relationship instability included individuals who
had divorced, separated, had more than one marriage (excluding widowers), or had ended a
cohabiting relationship (defined as living with someone for over 6 months).
Sample Propensity Weights
As described above, the sample of twins for whom offspring data are available (the Children
of Twins sample) was selected from a larger, volunteer twin sample (the 1992 study). To
avoid bias in parameter estimates and standard errors introduced by sample selection, we
constructed a set of sample propensity weights for the twin data using the procedure outlined
in Heath, Madden, and Martin (1998). First, we identified predictors of whether one or both
twins from a pair from the larger sample participated in the Children of Twins sample.
Predictors of pairwise participation in the subsample were examined because selection
occurred at the twin-pair level. Propensity weights were then constructed using the inverse
probability of a pair participating in the Children of Twins sample. The ability of these
propensity weights to remove bias introduced by sample selection and nonrandom attrition
D’Onofrio et al. Page 6
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
was tested by comparing the weighted and unweighted distributions for demographic and
psychiatric traits in the subsample and the 1992 sample of the adult twins (results available
upon request; Harden et al., 2007). The lifetime risk of separation and multilevel linear
models described below were estimated using the sample propensity weights to approximate
population-based parameters and to avoid bias in standard errors.
Results
The Children of Twins Design is a three-level design (Nance & Corey, 1976). The 2,334
offspring were nested into 1,224 nuclear families, which were nested in 836 twin families.
Divorce status was missing for one twin in 14 of the families, influencing 24 offspring in the
study. These incomplete data, however, were not associated with offspring relationship
instability (odds ratios [OR] = 0.89, p = .90) or parental divorce status (OR = 0.81, p = .66).
As a result, the data were considered missing at random, and these twin families and
offspring were removed from the analyses, leaving 2,310 offspring in the analyses.
Our approach to analyzing the adult twin and Children of Twins data involved several steps.
First, survival analyses were conducted, ignoring the clustered nature of the data, to estimate
the risk of marital separation for offspring in intact and divorced families. Second, the risk
of relationship instability was calculated separately for offspring from twin families who
were discordant for divorce, which provides a comparison of cousins differentially exposed
to parental divorce. Third, the offspring of discordant twins were compared separately by
zygosity status. The comparison of offspring of identical twins controls for genetic risk
associated with parental divorce from the twin parents. Thus, the comparison of cousins
from identical twin families provides a more stringent test of causality, although it does not
account for every possible confound. No significance testing was done at this stage because
the estimates of risk are purely descriptive. Formal significance testing was completed using
models that could both address the nonindependence of the observations and include the
sample weights. Fourth, associations between measured parental characteristics and
offspring relational instability were estimated to identify covariates that would be included
in subsequent genetically informed models. Finally, multilevel models were used to
incorporate the measured characteristics of the parents and the quasiexperimental approach
available in the Children of Twins Design, as outlined in the introduction. The multilevel
models used in the current project are regression-based analyses that could account for the
nonindependence of the observations in the sample (e.g., multiple offspring per nuclear
family were included, which were nested under twin families). The multilevel models
estimated the magnitude of the intergenerational transmission comparing unrelated
individuals, offspring of all twins who were discordant for divorce, and offspring of
identical twin families who were discordant for divorce. The multilevel models also allowed
measured covariates to be included and weighted the analyses accordingly. Each model is
explained in detail below.
Association Between Parental and Offspring Relationship Instability Using Different
Comparisons
Survival analyses were used to explore the inter-generational association of marital
instability because of the right-censored nature of the offspring data (all the offspring have
not lived through the risk period for the outcome). These analyses, ignoring the nested
nature of the data, were conducted to provide initial estimates of the intergenerational
association. The offspring who experienced a divorce and separation were initially
compared to individuals in the entire sample, regardless of whether they had ever entered
into a cohabiting or married relationship. The analyses were also conducted on a restricted
subset of the data that only included offspring who had ever been in a relationship to
determine whether earlier initiation of intimate relationships accounted for the higher rate of
D’Onofrio et al. Page 7
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
separation in the offspring from divorced families, but the results did not change (results
available upon request).
For the entire sample, 10.3% (237/2,310) of the offspring had ever separated or divorced.
The weighted percentage, which was based on the propensity sampling weights, was 10.1%.
The risk for marital instability, controlling for the gender of offspring, was estimated using
Cox Regression Models (Allison, 1995) at the age of 32, the last age at which comparisons
could be made across all subgroups. The weighted estimated risk of separating, a KaplanMeier nonparametric estimate, was 18.1%. Table 1 presents the risk for relationship
instability, including the raw percentages, the weighted percentages, and the Kaplan-Meier
estimates, using multiple comparisons. Using the entire sample, offspring from intact
families had a lower risk of separating (16.0%) than offspring from divorced homes
(25.2%). The results replicate the established finding that offspring from divorced families
have higher rates of marital instability.
Instead of comparing offspring from unrelated families, the comparison of twins discordant
for divorce contrasted cousins (one set of cousins experienced the separation of their parents
but the other cousins did not), which provides a stronger test of causality because cousins
share traits that could confound the intergenerational transmission. If parental divorce
caused offspring marital instability, the relation would be present at all levels of analysis
(e.g., Rodgers et al., 2000). Offspring from the intact twin families had less risk of
separating (15.5%) than their cousins from divorced households (22.9%), a difference that is
similar in magnitude to the comparison of unrelated individuals, although perhaps a bit
smaller.
The prevalence and risk for offspring relationship instability were calculated separately for
offspring of identical and fraternal twins discordant for divorce. Comparison of the risks
among the groups provides an initial assessment of the processes responsible for the
intergenerational association between parental and offspring marital instability (for an
explanation of the rationale see D’Onofrio et al., 2003; Gottesman & Bertelsen, 1989). A
comparison of the offspring of identical twins discordant for divorce provides the strongest
evidence of whether selection factors are responsible for the intergenerational relation.
Offspring of identical twins who are discordant for divorce receive the same genetic risk
associated with divorce from the twins and shared environmental experiences that make the
adult twins similar. Therefore, differences between these two groups suggest that factors
within twin families associated with divorce account for the association, consistent with the
theory that divorce causes the difference between offspring in intact and divorced homes. In
contrast, if the magnitude of the difference in the offspring of the discordant identical twins
is less than the comparison of unrelated offspring, the pattern suggests that selection factors
are responsible for part of the intergenerational association. A comparison of offspring of
fraternal twins discordant for divorce provides a similar test, except these offspring differ
from the children of identical twins with respect to genetic risk associated with divorce from
the twin parents. Therefore, genetic factors are implicated if the divorce effect in the
offspring of fraternal twin families is larger than in the offspring of identical twins
discordant for divorce. If the magnitude of the divorce effect is equivalent in each type of
twin family and lower than the divorce effect estimated using unrelated offspring, common
environmental factors are implicated as selection factors.
Children of identical cotwins whose parents remained married had a 17.1% risk for divorce,
whereas children of identical cotwins whose parents divorced had a 22.0% risk for divorce.
The offspring of discordant identical twin pairs differed in their risk for divorce, consistent
with the hypothesis that the intergenerational association is causal, although the difference is
again somewhat smaller than was found in the comparison of unrelated individuals. The
D’Onofrio et al. Page 8
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
difference in risk in the offspring of fraternal twins discordant for divorce (16.9% vs. 24.1%)
appears to be larger than the difference in the offspring of identical twins discordant for
divorce. The pattern of results suggests that risk factors specifically associated with parental
divorce influence offspring risk for marital instability (the difference in marital stability
when comparing offspring of identical twins discordant for divorce). Genetic factors may
partially contribute to the risk of relationship instability in the offspring, however, because
(a) the divorce effect is smaller in identical twin families than the comparison of unrelated
individuals and (b) the divorce effect is larger in offspring of fraternal twins discordant for
divorce compared to offspring of discordant identical twins.
Associations Between Parental Characteristics and Offspring Relationship Instability
The Children of Twins Design cannot control for confounds that vary within twin families
(i.e., environmental influences that influence one twin and their offspring and the effects of
the spouses of the twins) and the approach assumes no assortative mating. Therefore,
association between characteristics of both parents and offspring relationship instability was
estimated in order to include measurements of potential confounds in subsequent genetically
informed analyses. Cox regression analyses with each individual parental characteristic were
conducted initially to identify characteristics of the parents associated with offspring
relationship instability and to reduce the number of variables included in the genetically
informative analyses. Because of the incomplete data on the mothers and fathers, the Cox
regression analyses were based on data sets in which the missing scores were estimated
through multiple imputation (MI) (Little & Rubin, 1987). As a result, the estimates of the
standard errors using MI reflect the uncertainty of the data resulting from the missing values.
More information was available for maternal versus paternal characteristics. The percentage
of missing values for mothers ranged from 4.2% to 8.5%, and the percentage of missing
values for fathers ranged from 11.0% to 24.5%. Because the nested nature of the data was
ignored in the analyses, the significance levels are biased downward. Therefore, the analyses
represent a liberal approach to identifying salient variables. Variables that were marginally
related to offspring relationship instability were used in the subsequent multilevel models.
The regression coefficients and OR associated with offspring relationship instability are
presented in Table 2. The results suggest that parental church attendance, age at first child,
history of conduct disorder, depression, and illegal drug use were associated with offspring
separation. These variables from both parents were included in the subsequent genetically
informed analyses.
Multilevel Models Utilizing Methodological and Statistical Controls
Multilevel models were conducted to quantify the magnitude of the association between
parental and offspring relationship instability using the different comparison groups within
the Children of Twins Design, as well as to control for measured characteristics of both
parents. The multilevel models accounted for the nested nature of the data and estimated risk
for offspring separation contingent on the offspring’s age to take into account the rightcensored data. The nonlinear multilevel models were fit with the HLM 6.0 software
(Raudenbush, Byrk, Cheong, Congdon, & du Toit, 2004). Extensive details and algebraic
models of the approach are explained elsewhere (D’Onofrio et al., 2005). We initially fit
three-level multilevel models (Raudenbush & Bryk, 2002) because the sample included
multiple offspring nested in nuclear families and two nuclear families nested in each twin
family. The multilevel model decomposed the residual variance in offspring relational
instability into random effects at each level. The limited variance common to all offspring in
a twin family (the highest level of analysis) caused estimation problems, however, and
subsequent analyses only included two levels. For simplicity, the variance component that
accounted for the nested nature of the data was the only random effect estimated in each
multilevel model. The fixed effect of offspring gender was included in each model. All
D’Onofrio et al. Page 9
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
models that incorporated statistical controls were conducted on five multiply imputed data
sets. Because of the estimation procedures used to fit nonlinear multilevel models in the
software program HLM, nested models cannot be compared using the standard model fitting
indices, such as changes in deviance (M. Du Toit, personal communication, September 6,
2006; Raudenbush et al., 2004). Therefore, our interpretations of the models are based on
the magnitude of the fixed effects associated with parental divorce, made with the
unstandardized coefficients (logits). Standardized estimates do not describe invariant causal
processes because they are influenced by factors unrelated to the specific relationship being
explored (Kim & Ferree, 1981). Standardized estimates (OR), however, were also reported
for descriptive purposes. We interpret the divorce effects cautiously on the basis of their
magnitude, rather than relying solely on statistical significance, because some comparisons,
such as the offspring of identical twins discordant for divorce, rely on relatively small
sample sizes (see Discussion).
Six multilevel models were fit; results are shown in Table 3. Model 1 included only the
phenotypic effect of experiencing parental divorce, the comparison of offspring from
divorced families to unrelated offspring in intact households. We refer to the estimate as
phenotypic because it represents the unadjusted association between two measured
characteristics and did not control for any genetic confounds. Parental divorce was
significantly associated with offspring relationship instability (b = .65 logits, SE = 0.19, OR
= 1.91, p < .001) in the first model, a finding that is consistent with previous research
showing that offspring from divorced families are more likely to divorce than offspring from
intact families. Model 2 included parental divorce (phenotypic) and measures of maternal
and paternal church attendance, age at the birth of their first child, conduct disorder,
depression, and use of illegal substances. The inclusion of the statistical controls for parental
characteristics slightly reduced the divorce effect, which remained significant (b = .52, SE =
0.22, OR = 1.68, p < .05). The parameters associated with the parental variables are difficult
to interpret because they estimate the unique contribution controlling for the presence of
every covariate in the model. The estimates of the influence of divorce, the divorce effect, in
each model are presented in Figure 2. The figure illustrates how using statistical controls of
characteristics of both parents in the second model, the standard approach for controlling for
possible confounds, slightly reduced the risk of offspring relationship instability related to
parental separation. The results suggest that psychiatric and demographic characteristics of
both parents account for only a minimal proportion of the intergenerational transmission of
divorce.
Model 3 separated the association between parental divorce and offspring relational
instability into between – and within – twin family effects. The effect of the average number
of divorces in a twin family (0, .5 [one twin divorced], or 1 [both twins divorced])
approximates the between-family effect of divorce (Jinks & Fulker, 1970). All cousins in a
twin family share this variable regardless of whether their parents divorced. The parameter
associated with variable (between), therefore, reflects whether individuals with more divorce
in their extended family have more marital instability than unrelated individuals who have
less parental divorce in their extended family. In contrast, the within –twin family effect
(within) compares cousins who were differentially exposed to parental divorce. The contrast
code to compare cousins of discordant twins was based on the difference between each
cotwin’s divorce status and the twin-family average. Offspring from twin families who were
either concordant for marital stability (both twins had not experienced divorce) or marital
instability (both twins had been divorced) received a value of 0 for the within – twin family
effect because there was no variation within twin pairs. The within – twin family variable
effectively compared offspring from the nondivorced cotwin (−.5) to their cousins, the
offspring of the cotwin who was divorced (.5). If parental divorce had a direct,
environmentally mediated effect of their offspring’s marital status, the association would be
D’Onofrio et al. Page 10
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
found within twin families, which measures the divorce effect using cousins instead of using
unrelated individuals as the comparison group. The within-family effect of divorce (within)
(b = .62, SE = 0.33, OR = 1.87, p = .06) in Model 3 was commensurate with the phenotypic
association reported in the first model and the between – twin family effect (between) (b = .
67, SE = 0.25, OR = 1.95, p < .05). Model 4 included the between- and within-family
estimates as well as the statistical controls for the parental characteristics, and the statistical
controls again reduced the association between parental and offspring relationship instability
(b = .54, SE = 0.34, OR = 1.71, p = .11). Figure 2 illustrates how the intergenerational risk
was slightly reduced when offspring whose parents were divorced were compared to their
cousins from intact families. The models still suggest, however, that parental divorce was
associated with offspring relationship instability when comparing cousins differentially
exposed to parental divorce and including measured characteristics of both parents in the
model.
Model 5 included the between-family estimate of divorce (the average of the number of
divorces of the two twins), but the within-family estimate of divorce was calculated
separately for the identical and fraternal twins. The strongest test of causality in the Children
of Twins Design is the comparison of offspring of identical twins. Plus, the difference in the
divorce effects measured in offspring of fraternal twins discordant for divorce compared to
discordant identical twins provides information on the nature of possible confounds (a larger
divorce effect in offspring of fraternal twins discordant for divorce suggests that genetic
factors account for part of the intergenerational association). Therefore, Model 5 included
the interaction between the within-family divorce variable and zygosity type, which was
coded identical = 0 and fraternal = 1. The multilevel models thus estimated two within –
twin family parameters: (a) the within – twin family effect in identical families and (b) the
difference in magnitude between the within – fraternal and the within –identical twin family
effects (bfraternal − bidentical). The comparison of cousins in identical twin families discordant
for divorce (within identical) was sizeable (b = .52, SE = 0.41, OR = 1.68, p = .21),
consistent with a causal relation between parent and offspring relationship instability. The
difference between the fraternal and the identical within estimate (within [fraternal −
identical]) (b = .22, SE = 0.64, p = .73) suggests that genetic factors may slightly confound
the inter-generational transmission of divorce, although the precision is limited by the
statistical power to estimate the parameter. Model 6 estimated the divorce parameters
included Model 5 and added the measures of parental characteristics. The identical withinfamily estimate (b = .43, SE = 0.40, OR = 1.55, p = .27) was slightly reduced, but the
difference in the two within-family estimates (b = .22, SE = 0.69, p = .75) remained the
same. Figure 2 shows how the within – twin family effects in fraternal twin families are
slightly larger than the estimates of the within – twin family effects in identical twin
families, again suggesting that genetic factors partially account for the inter-generational
transmission of divorce. Yet, the comparison of offspring of identical twins discordant for
divorce also revealed a sizeable divorce effect.
In summary, the multilevel models illustrate how the Children of Twins Design and the use
of statistical controls help account for selection factors. The magnitude of the association
between parental and offspring marital instability, however, remained generally robust
throughout, which is consistent with a causal relation. The initial multilevel model, which
estimated the unadjusted intergenerational relation, found that offspring from divorced
families had a higher likelihood of marital instability than offspring from intact homes (b = .
65 logits, OR = 1.91). The use of statistical controls for characteristics of both parents
slightly reduced the effect in Model 2. Models 3 and 4 compared cousins who were
differentially exposed to parental to account for unmeasured confounds. Models 5 and 6
estimated the effect of parental divorce in offspring of identical twins discordant for divorce
and whether the divorce effect was larger in fraternal twin than identical twin families. The
D’Onofrio et al. Page 11
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
divorce effect was slightly larger in fraternal families, suggesting that genetic factors shared
by parents and offspring partially account for the intergenerational transmission, but the
comparison of identical twins in the final model (b = .43 logits, OR = 1.55) is also consistent
with a causal association. To state it in a different way, 66% (.43 logits/.65 logits) of the
original, unadjusted estimate of the intergenerational transmission was not accounted for by
genetic confounds associated with divorce from the twin parents, nonmeasured
environmental confounds that make adult twins similar, and measured demographic and
psychological traits of both parents. Additional multilevel models that also controlled for
measures of parental personality factors, such as neuroticism, extra-version, and impulsivity,
in addition to those presented here, resulted in similar findings (results available by request).
Discussion
Consistent with extensive research on Western families (Pryor & Rogers, 2001), the current
analyses found a higher rate of relationship instability among the offspring of divorced
parents in a large sample of children of Australian twins, weighted to approximate
population-based parameters. The unique sample allowed us to conduct the first genetically
informed analysis of the intergenerational association. The current study also provides a
more rigorous test of causality than most previous studies because it included measured
covariates of both parents to control for selection factors, including lifetime history of
psychiatric disorders assessed by semistructured psychiatric interviews.
The results support both causal and selection processes (Hetherington, 1999). The use of the
Children of Twins Design revealed that parental divorce (or risk factors specifically
associated with parental marital instability within twin families) accounted for
approximately 66% of the initial, unadjusted estimate of the intergenerational transmission.
Because our design allowed us to control for genetic and unmeasured environmental
selection effects (for the twin parents) as well as measured characteristics of both parents,
this is perhaps the strongest evidence to date that parental divorce directly causes an increase
in offspring divorce.
Our results also suggest, however, that part of the intergenerational association
(approximately 34%) results from selection effects. Does the current study shed light on the
sources of the selection factors? Statistically controlling for characteristics of both parents
(Model 2) slightly reduced the overall magnitude of the association. Thus, families in which
both parents have higher levels of psychopathology are more likely to get divorced and have
children with higher levels of marital instability. Yet, only by comparing offspring of
identical twins discordant for divorce did the analyses more fully account for shared genetic
risk passed from parents to their children. The selection factors that were controlled by (a)
the use of the statistical covariates in Model 2 and (b) the comparison of offspring of
identical twins discordant for divorce in Model 5 appear to be largely independent because
the divorce effect in the model utilizing both approaches (Model 6) provided a smaller
estimate than when using either approach alone. Therefore, the measured covariates that
were included in the model—parental church attendance, age at first child, conduct disorder,
depression, and illegal drug use—do not mediate or explain the mechanisms through which
genetic selection factors account for some of the intergenerational transmission of divorce.
As such, the present analyses reveal only the broad, not the specific, genetic contributions to
the increased risk of marital instability among offspring of divorced parents. Unspecified
genetic factors partially account for an increased risk of divorce both in parents and in their
children. These results are consistent with concerns raised earlier about refining theories of
the consequences of divorce for children (Emery, 1999; Hetherington, 2003). We suspect
that one reason for this is that the results are attributable to multiple genetically mediated
D’Onofrio et al. Page 12
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
mechanisms. These likely include personality traits, broadly construed (Emery et al., 1999;
Jockin, McGue, & Lykken, 1986), and also physical characteristics such as early menarche,
a highly heritable developmental landmark (Mustanski, Viken, Kaprio, Pulkkinen, & Rose,
2004). Genetic selection effects also may be partially mediated through the environmental
experiences. For example, adolescent girls whose early menarche is attributable to genetics
may develop secondary sexual characteristics that attract the attention of older, perhaps
manipulative men who are less stable or reliable romantic partners, and this experience, in
turn, leads to more divorce. Future research will need to explore the precise environmental
and genetic processes that mediate the intergenerational association by examining the traits
and personality characteristics of the offspring.
Concerns about genetic selection are not merely academic. Whether parental divorce is
causal or merely correlated with offspring marital stability is tremendously important not
just conceptually but also practically, especially given the increase in divorce rates in the
United States and in other Western countries in the past 50 years (Pryor & Rogers, 2001)
and the current debate over government involvement to promote marriages and reduce
divorces (McLanahan, Donahue, & Haskins, 2005). The present results imply that reducing
the incidence of parental divorce or ameliorating the risk factors that frequently accompany
the separation of parents (e.g., increased financial difficulties, parenting problems, lower
commitment to marriage) may reduce the risk of marital instability in offspring. A number
of intervention studies with residential mothers and children from divorced families have
reduced the risk of mental health problems in offspring (e.g., Wolchik et al., 2002), but no
study has explored whether interventions can reduce the increased risk of relationship
instability in the offspring of divorced parents.
A number of limitations must be considered when interpreting the results. Genetic and
environmental effects may well differ across cultures, cohorts, and time. Historical and
cross-cultural evidence clearly indicates broad environmental influences on divorce
thresholds. Like others, our research shows that genetic factors influence divorce risk within,
not between, culture and cohort. The proportions of the variance attributable to genetic
selection and environmental causation are not fixed, especially in accounting for complex
social behaviors such as divorce. Thus, the present results may, or may not, apply to intergenerational linkages found in the United States or other Western countries.
The study utilized sampling weights and statistical techniques to handle missing data (MI)
so that the results would generalize to the larger population of Australia, but the present
results apply only to the Australia and are limited to offspring’s experience of divorce (and
the break up of long-term cohabiting relationships) early in adult life. Previous research,
however, suggests that the divorce rate in Australia is consistent with other non-U.S.
countries (Pryor & Rogers, 2001), and results of divorce studies in Australia are generally
commensurate with international studies (Rodgers, 1996). Nevertheless, the relative
importance of genetic and environmental processes responsible for the intergenerational
transmission of divorce may change when considering risk of marital instability throughout
the entire life span. Our analytical approach of handling the right censoring may not
accurately account for the intergenerational association if different factors contribute to
relationship instability in early versus later adult life.
Although the study is the first to examine genetic selection and also included many other
potential selection factors, the study is still based on correlational data. Therefore, the results
are limited by the constraints inherent in using any nonexperimental design and cannot
prove that parental divorce causes offspring relationship instability (D’Onofrio et al., 2003;
Rutter et al., 2001). Parental divorce could be a proxy for other environmental risk factors
that actually cause the offspring to get divorced. As discussed above, the Children of Twins
D’Onofrio et al. Page 13
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Design does not control for genetic risk from the spouse of twins (Eaves et al., 2005). The
analyses controlled for characteristics of both parents to address this limitation, but it is
impossible to determine whether the measured covariates account for all the relevant
characteristics. Furthermore, characteristics of the offspring that increase the likelihood of
divorce in both generations could also explain the relation (reciprocal influences).
There are also methodological limitations to note. Some caution is warranted because the
standard errors around many estimates were large. The Children of Twins Design also
assumes the absence of assortative mating (Heath et al., 1985). Assortative mating, the
tendency for individuals to marry similar people, may place some children at greater risk for
marital separations because the offspring are exposed to two parents with increased levels of
psychopathology and other characteristics that are deleterious for offspring. Understanding
the exact mechanisms underlying assortative mating, whether driven by genetic or
environmental processes, is therefore required to specify the risk mechanisms responsible
for the association between parental and offspring characteristics (Heath et al., 1985). If
assortative mating is driven by genetic factors (e.g., Eaves, 1979), some offspring would
inherit greater genetic marital stability. Assortative mating, however, may also have
environmentally mediated effects on offspring, through processes such as modeling
(D’Onofrio et al., 2003). A thorough analysis of the processes responsible for assortative
mating in the current sample of twins and their spouses is beyond the scope of the current
manuscript, but other articles using the same sample are beginning to address the question
(e.g., Agrawal et al., 2006). We are currently working with other researchers on models that
can incorporate the influence of spouses and the mechanisms responsible for mate selection.
A number of recommendations can be made for future studies exploring the
intergenerational transmission of divorce. Large-scale genetically informed studies need to
be conducted with sizable, nationally representative samples to study divorce and other
topics in marriage and families. Larger samples sizes would provide greater statistical
precision around the estimates, allow comparisons among different ethnic and
socioeconomic groups, enable further exploration of moderators of the intergenerational
association, and include measures of relationship instability across the life span. Additional
genetically informative designs, such as adoption studies, must also be used because
identifying causal environmental processes will require corroborating evidence from
multiple research approaches, each with its own strengths and weaknesses.
More generally, the present analyses together with additional, genetically informed studies
of the consequences of divorce (D’Onofrio et al., 2005, 2006, in press), stepfather presence
(Mendle et al., 2006), parental abuse (Jaffee, Caspi, Moffitt, & Taylor, 2004), parental
conflict (Harden et al., 2007), parenting characteristics (Caspi et al., 2004; Neiderhiser et al.,
2004), and severe parental psychopathology (Gottesman & Bertelsen, 1989), indicate that
demographers, sociologists, economists, and others studying population groups need to
recognize the importance of genetic selection and control for genetic effects using twin and
similar family research designs (Rutter et al., 2001).
In summary, the first genetically informed study of the intergenerational transmission of
divorce suggests that genetic factors, a risk factor that has not been explored in many family
studies, partially account for the association between parental and offspring relationship
instability. At the same time, the findings are consistent with the importance of
environmentally mediated risk for offspring divorce. The findings contrast with theories that
postulate that family environmental experiences in general (Harris, 1998; Rowe, 1994), and
parental divorce in particular (Begley, 1998), do not influence offspring adjustment after the
children leave home. Ultimately, exploring the influence of parents and family risk factors
will require research that integrates the underlying genetic and environmental processes.
D’Onofrio et al. Page 14
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Acknowledgments
The analyses were supported by a grant to the first author from the National Institute of Mental Health (MH67300).
Data collection was funded by grants from the W. T. Grant Foundation, NIAAA (National Institute on Alcohol
Abuse and Alcoholism) (AA07535 and AA000264) and the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and
Depression. The authors thank the staff of the Genetic Epidemiology Unit at QIMR (Queensland Institute of
Medical Research) for the data collection, in particular Alison Mackenzie for coordination, and the twins and their
children for cooperation in this research. Special thanks also to Mathilda du Toit for help with the analyses.
References
Agrawal A, Heath A, Grant J, Pergadia M, Statham D, Bucholz K, Martin NG, Madden PAF.
Assortative mating for cigarette smoking and for alcohol consumption in female Australian twins
and their spouses. Behavior Genetics 2006;36:553–566. [PubMed: 16710775]
Allison, PD. Survival analysis using SAS: A practical guide. Cary, NC: SAS; 1995.
Amato, PR. Children of divorced parents as young adults. In: Hetherington, EM., editor. Coping with
divorce single parenting and remarriage: A risk and resiliency perspective. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum;
1999. p. 147-163.
Amato PR. The consequences of divorce for adults and children. Journal of Marriage and the Family
2000;62:1269–1287.
Begley S. The parent trap. Newsweek 1998 September 7;:52–59.
Booth A, Carver K, Granger DA. Biosocial perspectives on the family. Journal of Marriage and the
Family 2000;62:1018–1034.
Bouchard TJ Jr, Loehlin JC. Genes, evolution, and personality. Behavior Genetics 2001;31:243–273.
[PubMed: 11699599]
Bucholz KK, Cadoret R, Cloninger CR, Dinwiddie SH, Hesselbrock VM, Nurnberger JI Jr, Reich T,
Schmidt I, Schuckit MA. A new semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage
studies: A report on the reliability of the SSAGA. Journal of Studies on Alcohol 1994;55:149–158.
[PubMed: 8189735]
Capaldi DM, Patterson GR. Relation of parental transitions to boys’ adjustment problems: I. A linear
hypothesis. II. Mothers at risk for transitions and unskilled parenting. Developmental Psychology
1991;27:489–504.
Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Morgan J, Rutter M, Taylor A, Arseneault L, Tully L, Jacobs C, Kim-Cohen J.
Maternal expressed emotion predicts childrens’ antisocial behavior problems: Using monozygotictwin differences to identify environmental effects on behavioral development. Developmental
Psychology 2004;50:149–161. [PubMed: 14979757]
Collins WA, Maccoby EE, Steinberg L, Hetherington EM, Bornstein MH. Contemporary research on
parenting: The case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist 2000;55:218–232. [PubMed:
10717969]
D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer E, Eaves LJ, Corey LA, Berg K, Solaas MH, Emery RE. The role of the
Children of Twins design in elucidating causal relations between parent characteristics and child
outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2003;44:1130–1144. [PubMed: 14626455]
D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer EN, Emery RE, Maes HH, Silberg J, Eaves LJ. A Children of Twins
Study of parental divorce and offspring psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and
Psychiatry. in press.
D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer EN, Emery RE, Slutske W, Heath A, Madden PAF, Martin NG. A
genetically informed study of marital instability and its association with offspring
psychopathology. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2005;114:570–586. [PubMed: 16351381]
D’Onofrio BM, Turkheimer EN, Emery RE, Slutske W, Heath A, Madden PAF, Martin NG. A
genetically informed study of the processes underlying the association between parental marital
instability and offspring adjustment. Developmental Psychology 2006;42:486–499. [PubMed:
16756440]
Eaves L. Use of twins in the analysis of assortative mating. Heredity 1979;43:399–409. [PubMed:
575719]
D’Onofrio et al. Page 15
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Eaves LJ, Silberg JL, Maes HH. Revisiting the children of twins: Can they be used to resolve the
environmental effects of dyadic parental treatment on child behavior? Twin Research 2005;8:283–
290.
Emery, RE. Marriage, divorce, and children’s adjustment. 2. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 1999.
Emery RE, Waldron MC, Kitzmann KM, Aaron J. Delinquent behavior, future divorce, or nonmarital
childbearing, and externalizing behavior among offspring: A 14-year prospective study. Journal of
Family Psychology 1999;13:1–12.
Fine, MA.; Harvey, JH., editors. Handbook of divorce and relationship dissolution. Mahwah, NJ:
Erlbaum; 2006.
Gottesman II, Bertelsen A. Confirming unexpressed genotypes for schizophrenia. Archives of General
Psychiatry 1989;46:867–872. [PubMed: 2802925]
Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endo-phenotype concept in psychiatry: Etymology and strategic
intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry 2003;160:636–645. [PubMed: 12668349]
Greenstein TN. Gender ideology, marital disruption, and employment of married women. Journal of
Marriage and the Family 1995;57:31–42.
Harden KP, Turkheimer E, Emery RE, D’Onofrio BM, Slutske W, Heath A, Martin NG. Marital
conflict and conduct disorder in Children-of-Twins. Child Development 2007;78:1–18. [PubMed:
17328690]
Harris, JR. The nurture assumption: Why children turn out the way they do. New York: Free Press;
1998.
Heath AC, Bucholz KK, Madden PA, Dinwiddie SH, Slutske WS, Bierut LJ, Statham DJ, Dunne MP,
Whitfield JB, Martin NG. Genetic and environmental contributions to alcohol dependence risk in a
national twin sample: Consistency of findings in women and men. Psychological Medicine
1997;27:1381–1396. [PubMed: 9403910]
Heath AC, Kendler KS, Eaves LJ, Markell D. The resolution of cultural and biological inheritance:
Informativeness of different relationships. Behavior Genetics 1985;15:439–465. [PubMed:
4074271]
Heath AC, Madden PAF, Martin NG. Assessing the effects of cooperation bias and attrition in
behavioral genetic research using data-weighting. Behavior Genetics 1998;28:415–427. [PubMed:
9926611]
Heath AC, Martin NG. Genetic influences on alcohol consumption patterns and problem drinking:
Results from the Australian NHMRC twin panel follow-up survey. Annals of the New York
Academy of Sciences 1994;708:72–85. [PubMed: 8154691]
Hetherington, EM. Coping with divorce single parenting and remarriage: A risk resiliency perspective.
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum; 1999.
Hetherington EM. Intimate pathways: Changing patterns in close personal relationships across time.
Family Relations 2003;52:318–331.
Jaffee SR, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Taylor A. Physical maltreatment victim to antisocial child: Evidence
of an environmentally mediated process. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 2004;113:44–55.
[PubMed: 14992656]
Jardine R, Martin NG. Causes of variation in drinking habits in a large twin sample. Acta Geneticae
Medicae et Gemellologiae 1984;33:435–450. [PubMed: 6543278]
Jinks JL, Fulker DW. Comparison of the biometrical, genetical, MAVA and classical approaches to the
analysis of human behavior. Psychological Bulletin 1970;73:311–349. [PubMed: 5528333]
Jockin V, McGue M, Lykken DT. Personality and divorce: A genetic analysis. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology 1996;71:288–299. [PubMed: 8765483]
Karney BR, Bradbury TN. The longitudinal course of marital quality and stability: A review of theory,
method, and research. Psychological Bulletin 1995;118:3–34. [PubMed: 7644604]
Kim J, Ferree GD. Standardization in causal analysis. Sociological Methods and Research
1981;10:187–210.
Lake RE, Eaves LJ, Maes HH, Heath AC, Martin NG. Further evidence against the environmental
transmission of individual differences in neuroticism from a collaborative study of 45,850 twins
and relatives on two continents. Behavior Genetics 2000;30:223–233. [PubMed: 11105396]
D’Onofrio et al. Page 16
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Little, RJA.; Rubin, DB. Statistical analysis with missing data. New York: Wiley; 1987.
McGue M, Lykken DT. Genetic influence on risk of divorce. Psychological Science 1992;6:368–373.
McLanahan S, Donahue E, Haskins R. Marriage and child wellbeing. Future of Children 2005;15:3–
12.
Mendle J, Turkheimer E, D’Onofrio BM, Lynch SK, Emery RE, Slutske W, Martin NG. Family
structure and age at menarche: A children of twins approach. Developmental Psychology
2006;42:535–542.
Mustanski BS, Viken RJ, Kaprio J, Pulkkinen L, Rose RJ. Genetic and environmental influences on
pubertal development: Longitudinal data from Finnish twins at ages 11 and 14. Developmental
Psychology 2004;40:1188–1198. [PubMed: 15535766]
Nance WE, Corey LA. Genetic models for the analysis of data from the families of identical twins.
Genetics 1976;83:811–826. [PubMed: 986976]
Neiderhiser JM, Reiss D, Pedersen NL, Lichtenstein P, Spotts EL, Hansson K, Cederblad M,
Elthammer O. Genetic and environmental influences on mothering of adolescents: A comparison
of two samples. Developmental Psychology 2004;40:335–351. [PubMed: 15122961]
Pryor, J.; Rogers, B. Children in changing families: Life after parental separation. Boston: Blackwell;
2001.
Raudenbush, S.; Byrk, A.; Cheong, YF.; Congdon, R.; du Toit, M. HLM 6: Hierarchical linear and
nonlinear modeling. Lincolnwood, IL: Scientific Software International; 2004.
Raudenbush, SW.; Bryk, AS. Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods. 2.
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage; 2002.
Rhee SH, Waldman ID. Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis
of twin and adoption studies. Psychological Bulletin 2002;128:490–529. [PubMed: 12002699]
Rodgers B. Social and psychological well-being of children from divorced families: Australian
research findings. Australian Psychologist 1996;3:174–182.
Rodgers J, Cleveland H, van den Oord E, Rowe D. Resolving the debate over birth order, family size,
and intelligence. American Psychologist 2000;55:599–612. [PubMed: 10892201]
Rowe, DC. The limits of family influence: Genes, experience, and behavior. New York: Guilford;
1994.
Rutter M, Pickles A, Murray R, Eaves LJ. Testing hypotheses on specific environmental causal effects
on behavior. Psychological Bulletin 2001;127:291–324. [PubMed: 11393298]
Rutter, M.; Silberg, J.; Simonoff, E. Whither behavior genetics? A developmental psychopathology
perspective. In: Plomin, R.; McClearn, GE., editors. Nature, nurture, and psychology. Washington,
DC: American Psychological Association; 1993. p. 433-456.
Scarr S, McCartney K. How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype →
environment effects. Child Development 1983;54:424–435. [PubMed: 6683622]
Slutske WS, Heath AC, Dinwiddie SH, Madden PA, Bucholz KK, Dunne MP, Statham DJ, Martin
NG. Common genetic risk factors for conduct disorder and alcohol dependence. Journal of
Abnormal Psychology 1998;107:363–374. [PubMed: 9715572]
Statham DJ, Heath AC, Madden PA, Bucholz KK, Bierut LJ, Dinwiddie SH, Slutske WS, Dunne MP,
Martin NG. Suicidal behaviour: An epidemiological and genetic study. Psychological Medicine
1998;28:839–855. [PubMed: 9723140]
Wolchik SA, Sandler IN, Millsap RE, Plummer BA, Greene SM, Anderson ER, Dawson-McClure SR,
Hipke K, Haine RA. Six-year follow-up of preventive interventions for children of divorce: A
randomized controlled trial. Journal of the American Medical Association 2002;16:1874–1881.
[PubMed: 12377086]
D’Onofrio et al. Page 17
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 1.
Environmental and Genetic Confounds to Intergenerational Relations.
Note: Reciprocal effects are not considered in the current model. The direct path between
the parental and the child trait is referred to as the quasicausal effect because it is consistent
with a causal association but does not prove causality.
D’Onofrio et al. Page 18
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
Figure 2.
Association Between Parental and Offspring Relationship Instability Using Methodological
and Statistical Controls.
Note: Cousins = offspring of twins, regardless of zygosity. DZ = dizygotic or fraternal twin
families. MZ = monozygotic or identical twin families.
1Statistical covariates (maternal and paternal church attendance, age at first child, conduct
disorder, depression, and illegal drug use) were included in the models.
D’Onofrio et al. Page 19
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
D’Onofrio et al. Page 20
Table 1
Risk for Relationship Instability in Offspring Using Multiple Comparison Groups
Family Structure Raw % Weighted % Weighted Estimated Risk n
Entire sample
Parents married 9.2 8.8 16.0 1,749
Parents divorced 13.6 14.5 25.2 561
Discordant twin families
Parents married 10.3 8.7 15.5 464
Parents divorced 12.3 12.6 22.9 382
Discordant fraternal twin families
Parents married 10.8 9.1 16.9 240
Parents divorced 9.1 12.2 24.1 176
Discordant identical twin families
Parents married 9.8 8.3 17.1 224
Parents divorced 15.1 12.1 22.0 206
Note: Weighted estimates were based on analyses using the propensity sampling weights. Estimated risks are based on Kaplan-Meier
nonparametric survival analyses at the age of 32, controlling for the gender of the offspring.
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
D’Onofrio et al. Page 21
Table 2
Parental Characteristics Associated With Offspring Divorce/Separation Status
Mother Father
Variables b OR p b OR p
Education −.08 0.92 .26 −.06 0.94 .45
Church attendance
a −.10 0.90 .15 −.12 0.89 .12
Age at first child
a −.08 0.92 <.01 −.07 0.93 <.01
Cigarette smoking .07 1.07 .70 .14 1.15 .59
Conduct disorder
a .14 1.15 .09 .11 1.12 .16
Alcohol abuse .06 1.06 .81 .17 1.19 .18
Depression
a .06 1.06 .04 .03 1.03 .47
Drug use
a .72 2.05 .05 .54 1.71 .10
Suicidality .08 1.08 .26 .17 1.19 .17
Note: All analyses are based on five multiply imputed data sets.
N = 1,210. The coefficients are based on Cox Proportional Hazard Models using multiple imputation, ignoring the nested nature of the data,
and are distributed as logits. OR = odds ratios. See text for units of measurement for each variable.
aIncluded in subsequent multilevel models predicting offspring divorce/separation status.
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.
NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript
D’Onofrio et al. Page 22
Table 3
Parameter Estimates From the Multilevel Models
Models
1 2 3 4 5 6
Parameter b SE b SE b SE b SE b SE b SE
Divorce
Phenotypic .65
* 0.19 .52
* 0.22
Between .67
* 0.25
* .50 0.27 .68
* 0.25 .50 0.27
Within .62 0.33 .54 0.34
Within identical .52 0.42 .43 0.40
Within (fraternal-identical) .22 0.65 .22 0.69
Offspring gender −.30 0.20 −.26 0.20 −.29 0.19 −.26 0.20 −.29 0.19 −.26 0.20
Maternal variables
Church attendance .05 0.07 .05 0.07 .05 0.07
Age at first child −.07 0.05 −.07 0.05 −.07 0.05
Conduct disorder .03 0.15 .04 0.15 .03 0.15
Depression .02 0.03 .02 0.03 .02 0.03
Illegal drug use −.34 0.33 −.34 0.33 −.34 0.33
Paternal variables
Church attendance −.07 0.09 −.07 0.09 −.07 0.09
Age at first child .01 0.04 .01 0.04 .02 0.04
Conduct disorder −.24 0.14 −.24 0.14 −.24 0.15
Depression .09
* 0.04 .09
* 0.04 .09
* 0.04
Illegal drug use −.35 0.22 −.35 0.23 −.35 0.23
Note: All parameters are unstandardized.
N = 2,310. Data are weighted. Models including the measured covariates are based on five multiply imputed data sets. Model 1 compares unrelated offspring. Model
2 includes statistical covariates to the model. Model 3 separates the influence of parental divorce into between – twin families and within – twin family (offspring of discordant twins) effects. Model 4
includes statistical covariates to the model. Model 5 calculates the within – twin family effect for identical twin families and the difference between the fraternal and the identical within – twin family effects.
Model 6 includes statistical covariates to the model.
*
p < .05.
J Marriage Fam. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 July 16.

Published by
Essays
View all posts